Skip to main content

Getting specific: participation preference in urooncological decision-making

Abstract

Background

Shared decision-making is the gold standard for good clinical practice, and thus, psychometric instruments have been established to assess patients’ generic preference for participation (e.g., the Autonomy Preference Index, API). However, patients’ preferences may vary depending on the specific disease and with respect to the specific decision context. With a modified preference index (API-Uro), we assessed patients’ specific participation preference in preference-sensitive decisions pertaining to urological cancer treatments and compared this with their generic participation preference.

Methods

In Study 1, we recruited (N = 469) urological outpatients (43.1% urooncological) at a large university hospital. Participation preference was assessed with generic measures (API and API case vignettes) and with the disease-specific API-Uro (urooncological case vignettes describing medical decisions of variable difficulty). A polychoric exploratory factor analysis was used to establish factorial validity and reduce items.

In Study 2, we collected data from N = 204 bladder cancer patients in a multicenter study to validate the factorial structure with confirmatory factor analysis.

Differences between the participation preference for different decision contexts were analyzed.

Results

Study 1: Scores on the specific urooncological case vignettes (API-Uro) correlated with the generic measure (r = .44) but also provided incremental information. Among the disease-specific vignettes of the API-Uro, there were two factors with good internal consistency (α ≥ .8): treatment versus diagnostic decisions. Patients desired more participation for treatment decisions (77.8%) than for diagnostic decisions (22%), χ2(1) = 245.1, p ≤ .001.

Study 2: Replicated the correlation of the API-Uro with the API (r = .39) and its factorial structure (SRMR = .08; CFI = .974). Bladder cancer patients also desired more participation for treatment decisions (57.4%) than for diagnostic decisions (13.3%), χ²(1) =84, p ≤ .001.

Conclusions

The desire to participate varies between treatment versus diagnostic decisions among urological patients. This underscores the importance of assessing participation preference for specific contexts. Overall, the new API-Uro has good psychometric properties and is well suited to assess patients’ preferences. In routine care, measures of participation preference for specific decision contexts may provide incremental, allowing clinicians to better address their patients’ individual needs.

Peer Review reports

Background

Shared decision-making (SDM) is now considered the gold standard for good patient-physician interaction [1] and a stipulated goal for quality improvement in most modern healthcare systems [2, 3]. SDM emphasizes the importance of the patient’s voice and embraces their right to self-determination, and is thus an ethical imperative [4]. Over the last two decades, evidence has emerged that patient participation in medical decision-making can yield positive effects [5], such as lower emotional distress for the patient [6, 7], higher satisfaction with the decisions, better interaction with the physician, and improved knowledge [8,9,10].

SDM is especially relevant for preference-sensitive decisions [11], where the most appropriate decision can only be determined by taking the patient’s preferences into account [12]. Such preference-sensitive decisions need to be made when there is scientific uncertainty, i.e., different treatment options are from a professional perspective in equipoise (equally effective) or when the decision substantially affects patients’ subsequent quality of life [13,14,15].

Patients in urooncological care are regularly confronted with demanding and ambiguous, thus preference-sensitive decisions. For example, treatment of early-stage prostate cancer requires patients and physicians to jointly deliberate multiple treatment options in equipoise: active surveillance, surgery, and several forms of radiation therapy [11, 16]. Another prime example of preference-sensitive decisions in urooncology is the choice of urinary diversion after cystectomy (the surgical removal of the bladder) [17], where only a small fraction of patients receives a clear cut-recommendation for a specific type of urinary diversion based on their medical parameters [18, 19]. Given the inherent preference-sensitive nature of urooncological decisions and the promising evidence for the positive effects of patient participation in these decisions, international urooncological treatment guidelines now explicitly recommend investing in patient participation [20, 21].

On the other hand, not all patients seek autonomy in medical decision-making to the same degree [22,23,24]; some authors even described a desire for dependency in some of their patients [25]. Either way, patients’ preference for autonomy varies substantially, depending on the context of a decision. For example, participation preference wanes with more severe diagnoses in case vignettes of varying illness severity [23]. A recent study found considerable differences between the desire for autonomy in generic medical decision-making and disease-specific decisions [26]. Thus, there is a need for psychometrically sound measures to assess patients’ desire for autonomy in the specific decision-making context of the treatment decisions they face, e.g., for the highly preference-sensitive decisions in the context of urological cancer treatment.

Previous research has identified at least two relevant dimensions of patients’ desire for autonomy: A) the preference for information-seeking and B) the preference for participation in decision-making [23]. While many studies have documented that patients unanimously have a high preference for information-seeking [27, 28], there are considerable inter and intra-individual differences in the preference for active participation in the decision-making process (i.e., participation preference).

Patients’ participation preferences have previously been assessed with several measures in various samples from many cultures, and findings have been systematically aggregated in seven reviews from 1998 to 2021 [27, 29,30,31,32,33,34]. Concerning urooncology, these reviews included nine studies on prostate cancer populations, with the amount of patients’ desiring active participation ranging from 42% [35] to 92.5% [36], and four studies on the decision context of prostate cancer screening, with the proportion of preference for active participation ranging from 64.4% [37] to 81% [38].

Given the previously outlined high variability in participation preference for different decision contexts and diagnoses, it appears imperative to assess participation preference in the specific decision context. Therefore, in light of the highly preference-sensitive decisions urooncological patients face, we set out to develop a measure to determine preference for participation in the specific context of the most important urooncological treatment decisions.

Currently, patients’ preferences for participation in decision-making are not routinely collected at the outset of urooncological treatments. However, such an assessment could inform individually tailored healthcare and pave the way for high-quality decisions [39]. Practitioners could be enabled to involve their patients in the decision-making process according to their true preferences rather than relying on assumptions derived from patients’ sociodemographic characteristics [40].

The consistent finding that physicians across disciplines appear to have difficulties to accurately predict their patients’ participation preferences highlights the benefit of a formal assessment with disease and context-specific questionnaires [41,42,43]. An accurate assessment of participation preference is a prerequisite for a proper SDM process. When patients feel obliged to exercise more autonomy than they prefer, it could induce or exacerbate negative feelings, such as being overwhelmed, afraid, or abandoned, and lead to significant decision regret [25, 33]. Assessing patients’ participation preferences for the specific decision context and prior to the decision situation could aid physicians in navigating how much involvement their patients want and prevent overwhelming the patients [44].

The primary endpoint of this research was to capture disease-specific participation preference and determine its potential incremental value. To this end, we developed an assessment instrument to measure specific preferences in the context of urooncological decision-making. After confirming its incremental value and good psychometric properties, it will be available for clinical practice. In order to increase the usefulness of the instrument for clinical practice, we took great care to devise a short and easy to comprehend scale. Moreover, we collected data from two populations in two independent studies in order to examine generalizability respectively usefulness in a broad range of clinical settings.

Although there are some studies on the autonomy preference in prostate cancer patients, as summarized above, there are, to the best of our knowledge, no studies about the autonomy preference in other common urooncological entities (e.g., bladder cancer). This lack of research is striking, given the imperative to implement SDM in urooncology due to the many preference-sensitive decisions these patients face. Thus, a secondary endpoint of our study was to contribute to the knowledge base on participation preference in previously neglected populations.

Methods

Samples and procedures

Urological cohort (Study 1)

Data collection was carried out between May 2019 and February 2021 as part of an SDM implementation study in general urological practice. We collected data at the outpatient clinic of the Department of Urology and Urosurgery at University Medical Center Mannheim, Germany. Eligible patients were at least 18 years old and fluent in German. While waiting for their consultation with the physician, they were informed about the study by a nurse. After providing informed consent, they were then asked to fill in a set of questionnaires.

Patients filled in the German Autonomy Preference Index (API; [45]), the API’s original vignettes (API-OV; [23]), and the newly developed urooncological case vignettes (API-Uro; [46]; see Sect. "Development of the urooncological case vignettes (API-Uro)" and Additional file 1: Appendix A). Further questionnaires on emotional distress, decisional conflict, patients’ attitudes and beliefs, and perceived participation were assessed and analyzed elsewhere (see [47,48,49] for more details). The study protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the Medical Faculty of Mannheim, University of Heidelberg (MA-2019-635N).

We recruited N = 502 urological patients (age > 18). We excluded 33 patients (6.6%) with more than 40% missing values. The remaining 469 patients were primarily male (86.8%), of older age (62.4 ± 13.6 years), and most were German nationals (97.2%). Due to the absence of any systematic pattern of missing values (Additional file 1: Figures B.1 and B.2), we used median imputation for cases where only one item was missing per questionnaire. We imputed 24 values (5.1%) for the API-OV and 26 values (5.5%) in the API-Uro questionnaire.

Bladder cancer cohort (Study 2)

In addition to the methods described above, we obtained data in a multicenter study with bladder cancer patients scheduled for radical cystectomy before their pre-treatment consultation. Data were collected between September 2019 and February 2022 in the urology departments of six independent German hospitals. Inclusion criteria and procedure were identical to the urological cohort study (Study 1) and equivalent in all six study centers. A study nurse approached patients, and after giving informed consent, they completed a set of self-report measures, including the German API [45], the original case vignettes API-OV [23], and the urooncological case vignettes API-Uro [46]. More details on the study procedure and additional measured constructs can be found in Köther et al. [50]. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Medical Faculty of Mannheim of the University of Heidelberg (MA-2019-727N).

A total of N = 223 bladder cancer patients participated in the study. We excluded n = 19 patients with more than 40% missing data. Patients in the final sample (N = 203) were, on average, 68.4 ± 9 years old, and the majority were male (74.5%) and German nationals (87.3%). Again, visual inspection of the response pattern for the generic, original vignettes API-OV (see Additional file 1: Figure B.3) and the urooncological case vignettes (API-Uro, see Additional file 1: Figure B.4) did not show any systematic pattern of missing values. Thus, median imputation was applied for cases with only one missing value. We imputed 13 (5.9%) values in the API-OV and 10 (4.9%) in the API-Uro.

Measures

The primary endpoint of our study was to assess and evaluate the relevance of specific measures of participation preference. Concomitantly, we aimed for a rigorous psychometric evaluation of the urooncology-specific participation preference measure, which we assessed in two independent study populations (Study 1 and Study 2). Although populations and procedures differed, the same set of measures was employed in both studies to increase comparability. They are, thus, outlined once for both studies.

Sociodemographic and clinical data

We collected information on the following sociodemographic patient characteristics: age, gender, highest educational level, occupational status, relationship status, and cohabitation. Furthermore, we retrieved patients’ primary diagnoses from their electronic health records. For the urological cohort (Study 1), primary diagnoses were dichotomized into non-oncological vs. oncological. We further categorized oncological diagnoses according to the cancer entities (prostate cancer, renal cancer, bladder cancer, and others).

Preference for participation in decision-making

Autonomy Preference Index (API)

The API [23] is an established, well-validated questionnaire that has been used in multiple contexts to assess the generic participation preference of patients [51]. The German version of the API consists of eleven Likert-response type items, with a five-point scale ranging from 0 “strongly disagree” to 4 “strongly agree” [45]. The API has two subscales: the information-seeking subscale (API-is; seven items) and the decision-making preference scale (API-dm; four inversed items). Sum scores for both scales are built, and for better interpretation, min–max normalized to range from 0 to 100, with higher values indicating a stronger desire for autonomy.

Original case vignettes of the API (API-OV)

In the original English version of the API [23], the decision-making subscale is supplemented with three vignettes (upper respiratory tract illness, high blood pressure, myocardial infarction). One of the authors (BB) translated the vignettes, and an independent bilingual speaker not associated with the project back-translated them. Deviations from the back-translated version to the original were revised until a consensus was reached.

Each vignette contains three items (putative decisions). Patients were asked to indicate who should make the corresponding decision for each of the nine items. Options were presented with a five-point Likert response format from 1 “physician alone” to 5 “patient alone”. We calculated a sum score with all nine items and transformed it with min–max normalization to range from 0 to 100. Higher values are indicative of a preference for more decision-making autonomy.

Development of the urooncological case vignettes (API-Uro)

Based on the API-OV vignettes, we constructed a measure for specific urooncological participation preference. First, an expert panel created an initial pool of 18 items (putative decisions). Items (putative decisions) were formulated based on German cancer aid’s patient guidelines for urooncological cancer entities, available at the foundation’s website [52]. The panel then clustered the items to represent six essential steps from initial diagnosis, through critical treatment decisions, to aftercare. Next, we conducted multiple rounds of revisions and incorporated the feedback of four patients who participated in a pilot test. As a result, item complexity was reduced, the instructions were simplified, and redundant or ambiguous items were removed. The resulting preliminary questionnaire contained six vignettes corresponding to twelve items.

The response format remained identical to the API-OV. An English translation of the final version of the questionnaire used in this study is available in Additional file 1: Appendix A. We built a total sum score for the API-Uro and transformed it with min–max normalization to range from 0 to 100. A higher score indicates a greater desire to participate. In addition, we calculated sum scores for the two factors we obtained during the psychometric evaluation of the questionnaire described below.

Statistical analyses

We conducted drop-out analyses in both studies using either Pearson χ2 test, Fisher’s exact test, independent sample Student’s t-test, or, in case of homogeneity of variances, Welch’s two-sample t-test. The psychometric evaluation of the API-Uro questionnaire involved the following consecutive steps: determining the factorial validity with a polychoric exploratory factor analysis (EFA) (urological cohort; Study 1), validation of the established factor structure with confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) in the bladder cancer cohort (Study 2), assessment of reliability in terms of internal consistency (ordinal coefficient α), and evaluation of construct validity of the API-Uro (both studies).

All psychometric analyses described above building upon a correlation matrix between the Likert response-format items (parallel analysis, EFA and CFA, Cronbach’s α) were calculated with polychoric correlations. We followed best practice recommendations [53,54,55] and made the following informed choices: EFA using minimal residual (minres) extraction method, with an oblique rotation (oblimin). As there is no single best technique to determine the optimal number of factors to retain, we compared results from multiple methods [54, 55]. To this end, we report results for the scree test, parallel analysis, VSS criterion, and Velicer’s MAP test.

The adequacy of the data was determined by inspection of the polychoric correlation matrix. The majority of inter-item correlations should fall between 0.3 and 0.7 [56]. We further checked the eligibility of our data for EFA with the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure of sample adequacy (MSA; Cut-Offs 0.6 = mediocre, 0.7 = middling, 0.8 = meritorious, 0.9 = marvelous [57]) based on the polychoric correlation matrix as well as Bartlett’s test of sphericity.

We tested the factorial structure obtained in the first study (urological cohort; Study 1) with ordinal confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) in our data from the second study (bladder cancer cohort; Study 2). Parameters were estimated with the diagonally weighted least square (DWLS) estimator, which has been shown to be robust and can handle non-multivariate normal data [58,59,60]. The model is evaluated in terms of the following fit statistics: RMSEA, SRMR, CFI, and TLI. Reliability was reported in terms of ordinal coefficient α as an appropriate measure for internal consistency in ordinal data [61].

In order to analyze the difference between the desire to participate in different decision-making contexts obtained during factor analysis (API-Uro factors), we categorized patients as participators and delegators based on an established convention from the literature [30, 50, 62]: a score of ≤ 40 indicates the desire to delegate the decision (delegators), while a score > 40 indicates a preference for autonomy and participation in decision-making (participators). Differences in the proportion of participators between the two decision contexts (API-Uro factors) were analyzed with the McNemar χ2 test.

Where applicable, we calculated and reported effect sizes; the interpretation of these effect sizes refers to the taxonomy of Cohen [63]. All analyses were carried out in R version 4.1.1 [64], and data were preprocessed with the tidyverse R-packages [65]. We used the R-packages psych and datscience for the polychoric EFA [66, 67]. The CFA was conducted with the lavaan, and lavaanExtra R-packages [68, 69]. A complete list of all R-packages can be obtained in Additional file 1: Appendix C.

Results

Sample characteristics

Most patients in both samples were male, German nationals, retired, lived with their partners, and had children. The average age of patients in the urological cohort (Study 1) was 62.4 ± 13.6 years, and in the bladder cancer cohort (Study 2), 68.4 ± 9 years. Further details on both samples’ characteristics can be found in Table 1. The urological cohort (Study 1) contained 43.1% patients with an urooncological diagnosis (65.3% prostate cancer). All patients were diagnosed with bladder cancer in the bladder cancer cohort (Study 2).

Table 1 Sociodemographic characteristics

Drop-out analyses

We analyzed differences in sociodemographic variables between the final sample and patients who dropped out or had more than 40% missing values separately for both studies.

In the urological cohort (Study 1), the excluded patient (n = 33) contained significantly more non-German nationals (33.3% vs. 2.8%, χ2(1) = 63.1, p ≤ 0.001) with a moderate effect size of φ = 0.35, and more female patients (28% vs. 13%, χ2(1) = 63.1, p ≤ 0.035), with a small effect φ = 0.1. Moreover, we found differences in their educational level (p = 0.004, Fisher’s exact test). Excluded patients reported lower educational levels with a small effect size, Cramer’s V = 0.17. No significant differences existed in any other sociodemographic variable (all ps ≥ 0.08).

In the bladder cancer cohort (Study 2), there were no systematic differences between excluded patients (n = 15) and patients in the final sample (all ps ≥ 0.07).

Descriptive statistics of participation preference

Table 2 summarizes the descriptive statistics for the assessed participation preference measures. In both studies, patients scored very high on the information-seeking subscale of the API (API-is, Mstudy1 = 95.8 ± 8.2, Mstudy2 = 92.5 ± 11.6), and there was a ceiling effect. Patients scored in the moderate range for the measures of participation preference in decision-making (see Table 2). The total API-Uro scores were comparable to those obtained with the established generic API version and the original case Vignettes (API-OV). However, there were considerable differences between the factors (decision contexts), which can be differentiated with the API-Uro (see Sect. "Context specificity and urooncological participation preference").

Table 2 Descriptive Statistics for Participation Preference

For the heterogeneous sample collected in the urological cohort (Study 1), we found no difference between urooncological and non-oncological patients in any of the participation preference measures (independent sample t-tests, all ps ≥ 0.141).

Psychometric evaluation of the API-Uro

Factorial validity and reliability

We utilized the large dataset collected in the urological cohort (Study 1) to conduct a polychoric exploratory factor analysis (EFA) to assess the construct validity and reduce the number of ambiguous items in the API-Uro questionnaire. In preparation for the EFA, we first assessed the adequacy of our data. The polychoric correlations of the API-Uro items ranged between r = 0.23 and r = 0.89 (Md = 0.49); see Table 3. The majority of correlations for each item fell in the range between 0.3 ≤ r ≤ 0.7, indicating acceptable relatedness. Thus, no item was removed [56]. The data suitability was also confirmed by a meritorious KMO measure of sampling adequacy of 0.84 (Kaiser & Rice, 1974) and a significant Bartlett’s test, χ2(66) = 2871.8, p ≤ 0.001. All items had a KMO value of greater than 0.7 (range = [0.81; 0.90]); thus, no item was removed.

Table 3 Correlation Matrix and Descriptive Statistics for the API-Uro Items

The optimal number of factors to retain was determined by parallel analysis based on the polychoric correlations (five factors), scree test (two factors), VSS criterion (two factors), and Velicer’s Map test (one factor). The suggested five factors in the parallel analysis likely resemble over-factoring, which is a known problem for larger sample sizes [70] as the randomly generated Eigenvalues get too small. We decided to retain two factors in concordance with the unambiguous scree test (see Additional file 1: Figure D.1) and the VSS.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Confirmatory factor analysis of the API-Uro (Model 2) in the bladder cancer cohort (Study 2). Note. Loadings of the two latent factors (circles) on the seven manifest API-Uro items (squares). All coefficients are standardized. Loadings were extracted with a diagonally weighted least square estimator. UVi.k = Urological Vignette, i indicates the vignette, k the item on the respective vignette (e.g., UV5.1, 5th vignette 1.st item). After inspection of modification indices, the covariance between the residuals of items UV5.1 and UV5.2 was permitted in Model 2

Extraction of two factors with polychoric EFA with minimum residual (minres) extraction method and oblimin rotation explained 58.95% of the variance in the original twelve API-Uro items. The loadings of the items on the two factors are presented in Table 4. Two vignettes (UV2: “Form of therapy “ and UV6: “Palliative systemic therapy”) and their five corresponding items (UV2.1 – UV2.3, UV6.1, UV6.2) were eliminated due to substantial cross-loadings (≥ 0.3) of at least one item per vignette and in consolidation with the expert panel in order to shorten the questionnaire and increase its efficiency for practice.

Table 4 Loadings of API-Uro Items in Polychoric EFA

The two vignettes “(Neo)adjuvant chemotherapy” (UV3) and “Functional preservation versus oncological safety” (UV4), loaded solely on factor one, while the remaining two vignettes, “Preventive care” (UV1) and “Follow-up care” (UV5) loaded exclusively on factor two. In consideration of the content of the respective vignettes, the factors were consequently termed treatment decisions (factor one, with vignettes UV3 and UV4) and diagnostic decisions (factor two, with vignettes UV1 and UV5). Standardized ordinal coefficients α were acceptable for the factor treatment decisions (α = 0.79) and good for diagnostic decisions (α = 0.9), as well as for the total sum scale (α = 0.92).

The psychometric analysis described above resulted in a shortened and final version of the API-Uro comprised of seven decisions presented in four vignettes: preventive care, functional preservation vs. oncological safety, (neo-) adjuvant chemotherapy, and follow-up care (see Additional file 1: Appendix A for the English version).

Validation of the factorial structure of the API-Uro

In order to validate the established two-factor structure of the API-Uro, we calculated a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) with the diagonally weighted least square estimator in the multicenter sample of 204 patients from the bladder cancer cohort (Study 2). Our first model (Model 1) was constructed to represent the factorial structure obtained with the polychoric EFA. In Model 1, the items UV3.1, UV4.1, and UV4.2 were expected to load on the treatment decisions (factor one) and the items UV1.1, UV1.2, UV5.1, and UV5.2 to load on the diagnostic decisions (factor two), with no covariance between the residuals. The standardized factor loadings for Model 1 ranged between 0.63 (UV1.1) and 0.88 (UV5.1). However, the obtained fit statistics suggested the need to improve the model to fit the data more adequately (TLI = 0.907, CFI = 0.943, SRMR = 0.108, RMSEA = 0.121).

After inspection of the modification indices, a residual covariance between the items UV5.1 and UV5.2 of the vignette UV5 “Follow-up care” was permitted in Model 2. This addition seems plausible, considering the overlap between the phrasing of the two items (see Additional file 1: Appendix A). The standardized factor loadings for Model 2 ranged between 0.6 (UV4.2) and 0.78 (UV1.2), see Fig. 1. The two factors in Model 2 correlated with r = 0.52, p ≤ 0.001. The model fit statistics obtained for Model 2 showed improved and adequate model fit: CFI = 0.974, TLI = 0.954, RMSEA = 0.085, SRMR = 0.081.

Construct validity of the API-Uro

We assessed the construct validity of the disease-specific API-Uro and its two factors (i.e., decision context) with correlation analyses, with the well-established generic API questionnaire [45], as well as with the original case vignettes from the English API (API-OV) [23].

The first factor treatment decisions correlated moderately with the generic disease-unspecific API (rstudy1 = 0.39 and rstudy2 = 0.35), and with the original API vignettes API-OV (rstudy1 = 0.38 and rstudy2 = 0.33), all correlations where significant (p ≤ 0.001).

The second factor diagnostic decisions of the API-Uro, also correlated moderately with the generic API (rstudy1 = 0.33 and rstudy2 = 0.34), but strongly with the generic vignettes API-OV (rstudy1 = 0.63 and rstudy2 = 0.60). Again, all correlations were significant below a threshold of p ≤ 0.001.

Context specificity and urooncological participation preference

In order to establish the context specificity of the API-Uro, and highlight the relevance of assessing patients’ preference for active participation in the specific decision contexts, we compared the participation preference for treatment decisions vs. diagnostic decisions in both studies. Patients were categorized based on a taxonomy from the literature [30, 50, 62] into either delegators (participation preference ≤ 40) or participators (participation preference > 40). Table 5 illustrates the differences in participation preference depending on the decision context and the respective diagnoses of the patients.

Table 5 Participation Preference (API-Uro) of the Urological Cohort (Study 1) and the Bladder Cancer Cohort (Study 2)

In the Urological Cohort (Study 1), the proportion of self-identified participators was independent of the primary diagnoses; however, there were substantial differences in the proportion of participators between the specific decision context (i.e., if the decisions were pertaining to a treatment decision or a diagnostic decision). Overall, a majority (76.8%—85.2%) had a desire to participate when it came to urooncological treatment decisions. In contrast, in the context of diagnostic decisions, only about a quarter of patients (22%—29.6%) reported a desire to participate.

The pattern of a substantially (p ≤ 0.001) higher proportion of participators for treatment decisions than for diagnostic decisions was replicated in the Bladder Cancer Cohort (Study 2). Again, a majority of 57.4% self-identified as participators for treatment decisions as opposed to only 13.3% for diagnostic decisions.

Discussion and conclusions

The key to the successful implementation of shared decision-making (SDM) is to accurately identify patients’ desire to participate in decision-making [14, 71]. Especially in cases where the stakes are high (typical in the treatment of urological cancer) and when the best decision heavily depends on the patient’s preferences [16], there is a need for psychometrically validated measures of participation preference.

Our findings demonstrated the importance of assessing patients’ preference for participation in the specific decision context of the current treatment. Overall, most urological and urooncological patients desired active participation in treatment but not in diagnostic decisions, highlighting the relevance of context-specific assessment of patients’ preferences in the field.

Differences in the participation preference between the two decision contexts might be due to the subjectively perceived difficulty or stakes of the decisions they comprise. The observed higher participation preference for the more difficult and complex treatment decisions compared to the lower-stakes diagnostic decisions might, at first glance, appear to contradict previous findings of lower participation preference in vignettes with more severe diseases [23, 72, 73]. However, we argue that it is important to disentangle the influences of illness severity and decision difficulty on participation preference.

While there is some evidence for a lower participation preference in patients with more severe illness, more comorbidities, lower physical functioning, or health-related quality of life [32, 73,74,75], we did not find any relationship between participation preference and illness severity in our urological cohort (Study 1). Table 5 illustrates that oncological patients did not differ from their non-oncological counterparts.

However, we found a substantial effect of the specific decision contexts (treatment vs. diagnostic decisions) on participation preference. This is in line with a previous study in which participants differed significantly in their participation preference in generic medical decision-making compared to decisions regarding a specific disease [26]. Furthermore, O’Dell and colleagues [37] investigated the influence of different decision contexts for prostate cancer screening decisions (PSA-Tests). They reported small differences between the participation preference for decisions about acceptable risks and benefits (71.3% active participation) compared to the decision about what treatment option is selected (64.4% active participation).

Overall, the new disease and context-specific Autonomy Preference Index – Urooncology (API-Uro) has promising psychometric properties and is a suitable tool to assess patients’ desire to participate in two important urooncological decision contexts, namely, treatment decisions and diagnostic decisions. The two-factor structure obtained in the heterogeneous sample collected in the urological cohort (Study 1) explained a substantial proportion of the variance and was replicated with an independent sample from a multicenter trial (bladder cancer cohort; Study 2). Internal consistency for the factors was acceptable to good. The finding of moderate correlations with the established, generic (i.e., independent of the disease and decision context) API questionnaire indicates that the new context-specific API-Uro measures a related but not identical construct, thus, speaking for its convergent construct validity.

Context-specific measures of participation preference, therefore, provide important information and incremental value over generic ones.

Strengths and limitations

Our study’s strengths include providing data on the participation preference of previously understudied urological cancer populations (e.g., bladder cancer patients) obtained in a multicenter study. Furthermore, the rigorous psychometric evaluation of the new API-Uro questionnaire and the replication of its factorial validity in an independent multicenter sample support the overall quality of the instrument.

However, the model fit for the replication of the factorial structure (with the CFA) was not perfect. This is somewhat expected and a typical problem in factor analysis methodology due to the fixation of small factor patterns in exploratory factor analysis (omission of loadings < 0.3; [76]). We optimized the fit by permitting a covariance between the residuals of two items with high similarity on the diagnostic decisions subscale. Thus reaching acceptable to good model fit on most fit statistics [77].

One general criticism of the measurement of preference prior to the consultation in which high-stake decisions are to be made is that patients may not be capable of fully anticipating the task ahead. If they are currently not affectively aroused (in a “cold” state, as when feeling calm while filling in a questionnaire), it may be difficult to imagine being in an affective aroused state (in a “hot state”, as when feeling anxious during the decision-making). Patients might consequently underestimate the motivational influence of the hot state on their preferences (cold-to-hot empathy gap) [30, 78]. Asking patients for their participation preferences before they are entirely aware of the possible options also forces them to prejudge their preferences [13].

The chosen format of the API-Uro to assess participation preference and presenting options in a vignette format could hold some merit and help counteract this problem. In addition, the temporal proximity of preference assessment directly before the decision situation in the presented studies might attenuate the expected effect of empathy gaps. In future studies, a direct pre-post consultation comparison of context-specific participation preference might help elucidate how much such empathy gaps impact patients’ preferences.

Another limitation is the systematic drop-out of non-German nationals and patients with lower education in our urological cohort (Study 1). This is problematic as it could indicate a language or cultural barrier to enrollment in our study. However, this study’s overall drop-out rate was low (6.6%), and the pattern did not occur in the bladder cancer cohort (Study 2), so that we trust the results to be reasonably representative.

Conclusion

Truly individualized care does not mean that every patient should be pushed to participate in decisions on every aspect of their care. Respecting patient autonomy includes identifying patients who do not wish to participate and to accommodate this preference as well [79]. Thus, for SDM to be successful, it is essential to validly and reliably assess patients’ preference for participation [33, 80]. Indeed, we found substantial differences in patients’ participation preferences depending on the specific decision contexts. Patients’ participation preference was substantially higher for treatment than for diagnostic decisions. This showcases the incremental value context-specific measures of participation preference.

Since the positive effects of patient involvement in SDM may only be achieved when there is a reasonable match between the preferred extent of participation and the perceived level of involvement [81,82,83], a precise and specific measurement tool is critical.

The newly developed API-Uro has good psychometric properties and may prove a valuable tool for SDM research and clinical practice. It could enable healthcare providers and researchers to adequately assess urooncological patients’ desire to participate in the two major decision contexts: treatment and diagnostic decisions. Given the substantial difference in participation preference between these two decision contexts, we recommend calculating separate scores on the two factors rather than on a total sum score only. Integration of the psychometrically validated, disease and context-specific API-Uro in clinical practice could enable the involvement of patients in agreement with their preferences and thus to better meet their needs.

Availability of data and materials

The data and R code that support the findings of this study will be deposited on MADATA (University of Mannheim, https://madata.bib.uni-mannheim.de/) Research Data Repository (doi: https://doi.org/10.7801/419) and made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Abbreviations

API:

Autonomy Preference Index

API-dm:

Decision-making (subscale of the API)

API-is:

Information-seeking (subscale of the API)

API-OV:

Autonomy Preference Index – original case vignettes

API-Uro:

Autonomy Preference Index – urooncology

CFA:

Confirmatory factor analysis

CFI:

Comparative fit index

DWLS:

Diagonally weighted least squares

EFA:

Exploratory factor analysis

RMSEA:

Root mean square error of approximation

SDM:

Shared decision-making

SRMR:

Standardized root mean square residual

TLI:

Tucker–Lewis index

UVi.k :

Urological Vignette, i indicates the vignette, k the item on the respective vignette

References

  1. Barry MJ, Edgman-Levitan S. Shared decision making-the pinnacle of patient-centered care nothing about me without me. N Engl J Med. 2012;366:780–1. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMp1109283.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Härter M, Moumjid N, Cornuz J, Elwyn G, van der Weijden T. Shared decision making in 2017: International accomplishments in policy, research and implementation. Z Evid Fortbild Qual Gesundhwes. 2017;123–124:1–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.zefq.2017.05.024.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Joseph-Williams N, Lloyd A, Edwards A, Stobbart L, Tomson D, Macphail S, et al. Implementing shared decision making in the NHS: lessons from the MAGIC programme. BMJ. 2017;357:j1744. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.j1744.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Beauchamp TL, Childress JF. Principles of biomedical ethics. USA: Oxford University Press; 2001. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0035-9203(02)90265-8.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  5. Joosten EAG, DeFuentes-Merillas L, de Weert GH, Sensky T, van der Staak CPF, de Jong CAJ. Systematic review of the effects of shared decision-making on patient satisfaction, treatment adherence and health status. Psychother Psychosom. 2008;77:219–26. https://doi.org/10.1159/000126073.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Shay LA, Lafata JE. Where is the evidence? A systematic review of shared decision making and patient outcomes. Med Decis Mak. 2015;35:114–31. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X14551638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Vogel BA, Helmes AW, Hasenburg A. Concordance between patients’ desired and actual decision-making roles in breast cancer care. Psychooncology. 2008;17:182–9. https://doi.org/10.1002/pon.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Nakayama K, Osaka W, Matsubara N, Takeuchi T, Toyoda M, Ohtake N, et al. Shared decision making, physicians’ explanations, and treatment satisfaction: a cross-sectional survey of prostate cancer patients. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak. 2020;20:334. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12911-020-01355-z.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Kehl KL, Landrum MB, Arora NK, Ganz PA, van Ryn M, Mack JW, et al. Association of actual and preferred decision roles with patient-reported quality of care. JAMA Oncol. 2015;1:50. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaoncol.2014.112.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Luo H, Liu G, Lu J, Xue D. Association of shared decision making with inpatient satisfaction: a cross-sectional study. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak. 2021;21:25. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12911-021-01385-1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Wennberg JE. Unwarranted variations in healthcare delivery: Implications for academic medical centres. BMJ. 2002;325:961–4. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.325.7370.961.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Sepucha KR, Fowler Jr FJ, Mulley Jr AG. Policy support for patient-centered care: The need for measurable improvements in decision quality. Health Aff. 2004;23 SUPPL.2. https://doi.org/10.1377/hlthaff.var.54.

  13. Elwyn G, Edwards A, Kinnersley P, Grol R. Shared decision making and the concept of equipoise: The competences of involving patients in healthcare choices. Br J Gen Pract. 2000;50:892–7.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Charles C, Amiram G, Whelan T. Shared decision-making in the medical encounter: What does it mean? (or it takes at least two to tango). Soc Sci Med. 1997;44:681–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(96)00221-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Elwyn G, Frosch DL, Rollnick S. Dual equipoise shared decision making: Definitions for decision and behaviour support interventions. Implement Sci. 2009;4. https://doi.org/10.1186/1748-5908-4-75.

  16. Orom H, Biddle C, Underwood W, Nelson CJ, Homish DL. What is a “good” treatment decision? Decisional control, knowledge, treatment decision making, and quality of life in men with clinically localized prostate cancer. Med Decis Mak. 2016;36:714–25. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X16635633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Nuijens ST, Cobussen-Boekhorst H, Heesakkers JPFA, Witjes JA. Patient-reported outcomes and health-related quality of life after urinary diversions. Curr Opin Urol. 2021;31:574–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/MOU.0000000000000906.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Lee RK, Abol-Enein H, Artibani W, Bochner B, Dalbagni G, Daneshmand S, et al. Urinary diversion after radical cystectomy for bladder cancer: Options, patient selection and outcomes. BJU Int. 2014;113:11–23. https://doi.org/10.2217/fneb2013.13.18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Evans B, Montie JE, Gilbert SM. Incontinent or continent urinary diversion: how to make the right choice. Curr Opin Urol. 2010;20:421–5. https://doi.org/10.1097/MOU.0b013e32833c9661.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Tikkinen KAO, Dahm P, Lytvyn L, Heen AF, Vernooij RWM, Siemieniuk RAC, et al. Prostate cancer screening with prostate-specific antigen (PSA) test: A clinical practice guideline. BMJ. 2018;362:k3581. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.k3581.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Sanda MG, Cadeddu JA, Kirkby E, Chen RC, Crispino T, Fontanarosa J, et al. Clinically localized prostate cancer: AUA/ASTRO/SUO guideline. Part I: Risk stratification, shared decision making, and care options. J Urol. 2018;199:683–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.juro.2017.11.095.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Levinson W, Kao A, Kuby A, Thisted RA. Not all patients want to participate in decision making: A national study of public preferences. J Gen Intern Med. 2005;20:531–5. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1525-1497.2005.04101.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Ende J, Kazis L, Ash A, Moskowitz MA. Measuring patients’ desire for autonomy: Decision making and information-seeking preferences among medical patients. J Gen Intern Med. 1989;4:23–30. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02596485.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Shinkunas LA, Klipowicz CJ, Carlisle EM. Shared decision making in surgery: a scoping review of patient and surgeon preferences. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak. 2020;20:190. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12911-020-01211-0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Durif-Bruckert C, Roux P, Morelle M, Mignotte H, Faure C, Moumjid-Ferdjaoui N. Shared decision-making in medical encounters regarding breast cancer treatment: The contribution of methodological triangulation. Eur J Cancer Care (Engl). 2015;24:461–72. https://doi.org/10.1111/ecc.12214.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Köther AK, Siebenhaar KU, Alpers GW. Shared decision making during the COVID-19 pandemic. Med Decis Mak. 2021;41:430–8. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X211004147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Benbassat J, Pilpel D, Tidhar M. Patients’ preferences for participation in clinical decision making: A review of published surveys. Behav Med. 1998;24:81–8. https://doi.org/10.1080/08964289809596384.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Hamann J, Bieber C, Elwyn G, Wartner E, Horlein E, Kissling W, et al. How do patients from eastern and western Germany compare with regard to their preferences for shared decision making? Eur J Public Health. 2012;22:469–73. https://doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/ckr112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Hubbard G, Kidd L, Donaghy E. Preferences for involvement in treatment decision making of patients with cancer: A review of the literature. Eur J Oncol Nurs. 2008;12:299–318. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejon.2008.03.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Chewning B, Bylund C, Shah B, Neeraj K, Gueguen JA, Makoul G. Patient preferences for shared decisions: A systematic review. Patient Educ Couns. 2012;86:9–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2011.02.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Say RE, Murtagh M, Thomson R. Patients’ preference for involvement in medical decision making: A narrative review. Patient Educ Couns. 2006;60:102–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2005.02.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Noteboom EA, May AM, Wall E, Wit NJ, Helsper CW. Patients’ preferred and perceived level of involvement in decision making for cancer treatment: A systematic review. Psychooncology. 2021 https://doi.org/10.1002/pon.5750.

  33. Brom L, Hopmans W, Pasman HRW, Timmermans DR, Widdershoven GA, Onwuteaka-Philipsen BD. Congruence between patients’ preferred and perceived participation in medical decision-making: A review of the literature. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak. 2014;14. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6947-14-25.

  34. Tariman JD, Berry DL, Cochrane B, Doorenbos A, Schepp K. Preferred and actual participation roles during health care decision making in persons with cancer: A systematic review. Ann Oncol. 2010;21:1145–51. https://doi.org/10.1093/annonc/mdp534.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Davison BJ, Degner LF, Morgan TR. Information and decision-making preferences of men with prostate cancer. Oncol Nurs Forum. 1995;22:1401–8.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Davison BJ, Gleave ME, Goldenberg SL, Degner LF, Hoffart D, Berkowitz J. Assessing information and decision preferences of men with prostate cancer and their partners. Cancer Nurs. 2002;25:42–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/00002820-200202000-00009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. O’Dell KJ, Volk RJ, Cass AR, Spann SJ. Screening for prostate cancer with the prostate-specific antigen test: Are patients making informed decisions? J Fam Pract. 1999;48:682–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Woolf SH, Krist AH, Johnson RE, Stenborg PS. Unwanted control: How patients in the primary care setting decide about screening for prostate cancer. Patient Educ Couns. 2005;56:116–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2003.12.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Jerofke-Owen T, Garnier-Villarreal M, Fial A, Tobiano G. Systematic review of psychometric properties of instruments measuring patient preferences for engagement in health care. J Adv Nurs. 2020;76(8):1988–2004. https://doi.org/10.1111/jan.14402.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. De las Cuevas C, Peñate W. Preferences for participation in shared decision making of psychiatric outpatients with affective disorders. Open J Psychiatry. 2014;04:16–23. https://doi.org/10.4236/ojpsych.2014.41004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Mühlbacher AC, Juhnke C. Patient preferences versus physicians’ judgement: Does it make a difference in healthcare decision making? Appl Health Econ Health Policy. 2013;11:163–80. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40258-013-0023-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Elkin EB, Kim SHM, Casper ES, Kissane DW, Schrag D. Desire for information and involvement in treatment decisions: Elderly cancer patients’ preferences and their physicians’ perceptions. J Clin Oncol. 2007;25:5275–80. https://doi.org/10.1200/JCO.2007.11.1922.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Schaede U, Mahlich J, Nakayama M, Kobayashi H, Takahashi Y, Saito K, et al. Shared decision-making in patients with prostate cancer in japan: Patient preferences versus physician perceptions. J Glob Oncol. 2018;2018:1–9. https://doi.org/10.1200/JGO.2016.008045.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Keating NL, Guadagnoli E, Landrum MB, Borbas C, Weeks JC. Treatment decision making in early-stage breast cancer: Should surgeons match patients’ desired level of involvement? J Clin Oncol. 2002;20:1473–9. https://doi.org/10.1200/JCO.20.6.1473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Simon D, Kriston L, Härter M. Die deutsche modifizierte fassung des Autonomie-Präferenz-Index (API-dm) [The German modified version of the Autonomy Preference Index (API-dm)]. Klin Diagnostik und Eval. 2011;4:5–14.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Büdenbender B, Kriegmair MC, Köther AK, Grüne B, Michel MS, Alpers GW. Autonomy Preference Index - Uroonkologie (API-Uro). 2021.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Grüne B, Köther AK, Büdenbender B, Michel MS, Kriegmair MC, Alpers GW. Patients’ perspective on shared decision-making in urology: A prospective study at a university hospital. World J Urol. 2021;39:4491–8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00345-021-03794-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Köther AK, Alpers GW, Büdenbender B, Lenhart M, Michel MS, Kriegmair MC. Predicting decisional conflict: Anxiety and depression in shared decision making. Patient Educ Couns. 2021;104:1229–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2020.10.037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Büdenbender B, Köther AK, Grüne B, Kriegmair MC, Michel MS, Alpers GW. When attitudes and beliefs get in the way of shared decision making: A mediation analysis of participation preference. Heal Expect. 2022. https://doi.org/10.1111/hex.13699.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Köther AK, Büdenbender B, Grüne B, Holbach S, Huber J, von Landenberg N, et al. Different patients, different preferences: A multicenter assessment of patients’ personality traits and anxiety in shared decision making. Cancer Med. 2022;11:2999–3008. https://doi.org/10.1002/cam4.4667.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Scholl I, Van LMK, Sepucha KR, Elwyn G, Légaré F, Härter M, et al. Measurement of shared decision making - A review of instruments. Z Evid Fortbild Qual Gesundhwes. 2011;105:313–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.zefq.2011.04.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. German Cancer Aid. Patientenleitlinien [Patient Guidelines]. https://www.krebshilfe.de/informieren/ueber-krebs/infothek/infomaterial-kategorie/patientenleitlinien/.

  53. Costello AB, Osborne JW. Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: Four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Pract Assessment, Res Eval. 2005;10. https://doi.org/10.7275/jyj1-4868.

  54. Watkins MW. Exploratory factor analysis: A guide to best practice. J Black Psychol. 2018;44:219–46. https://doi.org/10.1177/0095798418771807.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Zygmont C, Smith MR. Robust factor analysis in the presence of normality violations, missing data, and outliers: Empirical questions and possible solutions. Quant Methods Psychol. 2014;10:40–55. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.10.1.p040.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Ferketich S. Aspects of item analysis. Res Nurs Heal. 1991;:165–8.

  57. Kaiser HF, Rice J. Little jiffy, mark IV. Educ Psychol Meas. 1974;34:111–7. https://doi.org/10.1177/001316447403400115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Rhemtulla M, Brosseau-Liard PÉ, Savalei V. When can categorical variables be treated as continuous? A comparison of robust continuous and categorical SEM estimation methods under suboptimal conditions. Psychol Methods. 2012;17:354–73. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029315.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Moshagen M, Musch J. Sample size requirements of the robust weighted least squares estimator. Methodology. 2014;10:60–70. https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-2241/a000068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Cheng-Hsien L. The performance of ML, DWLS, and ULS estimation with robust corrections in structural equation models with ordinal variables. Psychol Methods. 2016;21:369–87. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000093.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Zumbo BD, Gadermann AM, Zeisser C. Ordinal versions of coefficients alpha and theta for likert rating scales. J Mod Appl Stat Methods. 2007;6:21–9. https://doi.org/10.22237/jmasm/1177992180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Flynn KE, Smith MA, Vanness D. A typology of preferences for participation in healthcare decision making. Soc Sci Med. 2006;63:1158–69.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull. 1992;112:155–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. 2021.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Wickham H, François R, Henry L, Müller K. dplyr: A grammar of data manipulation. 2021.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Revelle W. psych: Procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research. 2021.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Büdenbender B. datscience: Data Science Utility Functions. 2021.

  68. Rosseel Y. lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012;48:1–36. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i02.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Thériault R. lavaanExtra: Convenience Functions for Package `lavaan`. 2022.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Warne RT, Larsen R. Evaluating a proposed modification of the Guttman rule for determining the number of factors in an exploratory factor analysis. Psychol Test Assess Model. 2014;56:104–23.

    Google Scholar 

  71. Makoul G, Clayman ML. An integrative model of shared decision making in medical encounters. Patient Educ Couns. 2006;60:301–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2005.06.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Nease RF, Brooks WB. Patient desire for information and decision making in health care decisions: The autonomy preference index and the health opinion survey. J Gen Intern Med. 1995;10:593–600. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02602742.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Colley A, Halpern J, Paul S, Micco G, Lahiff M, Wright F, et al. Factors associated with oncology patients’ involvement in shared decision making during chemotherapy. Psychooncology. 2017;26:1972–9. https://doi.org/10.1002/pon.4284.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Arora NK, McHorney CA. Patient preferences for medical decision making: Who really wants to participate? Med Care. 2000;38:335–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Longtin Y, Sax H, Leape LL, Sheridan SE, Donaldson L, Pittet D. Patient Participation: Current Knowledge and Applicability to Patient Safety. Mayo Clin Proc. 2010;85:53–62. https://doi.org/10.4065/mcp.2009.0248.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. van Prooijen J-W, van der Kloot WA. Confirmatory analysis of exploratively obtained factor structures. Educ Psychol Meas. 2001;61:777–92. https://doi.org/10.1177/00131640121971518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model. 1999;6:1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Loewenstein G. Hot-cold empathy gaps and medical decision making. Heal Psychol. 2005;24:S49-56. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.24.4.S49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Kiesler DJ, Auerbach SM. Optimal matches of patient preferences for information, decision-making and interpersonal behavior: Evidence, models and interventions. Patient Educ Couns. 2006;61:319–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2005.08.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Waterworth S, Luker KA. Reluctant collaborators: Do patients want to be involved in decisions concerning care? J Adv Nurs. 1990;15:971–6. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.1990.tb01953.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Jahng KH, Martin LR, Golin CE, DiMatteo MR. Preferences for medical collaboration: Patient-physician congruence and patient outcomes. Patient Educ Couns. 2005;57:308–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2004.08.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Gattellari M, Butow PN, Tattersall MH. Sharing decisions in cancer care. Soc Sci Med. 2001;52:1865–78. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00303-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Butow PN, Devine R, Boyer M, Pendlebury S, Jackson M, Tattersall MHN. Cancer consultation preparation package: Changing patients but not physicians is not enough. J Clin Oncol. 2004;22:4401–9. https://doi.org/10.1200/JCO.2004.66.155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all patients who participated in the two studies and the study nurses who supported the recruitment. The publication of this article was funded by the University of Mannheim.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. Financial support for this study was provided by the German Cancer Aid (Deutsche Krebshilfe) funding program Patient Orientation (grant number: 7011323). The funding agreement ensured the authors’ independence in designing the study, interpreting the data, writing, and publishing the report.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

BB substantially contributed to the conception and design of the study, acquisition of data, data analysis and interpretation, drafting and revising of the manuscript. AKK substantially contributed to the conception and design of the study, data interpretation, and revising of the manuscript. MCK substantially contributed to the conception and design of the study, revising of the manuscript. BG substantially contributed to the conception and design of the study, revising of the manuscript. MSM substantially contributed to the acquisition of the data and funding. GWA substantially contributed to the conception and design of the study, acquisition of funding, data interpretation, and revising of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Georg W. Alpers.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Both studies were approved by the ethics committee of the Medical Faculty of Mannheim, University of Heidelberg (Study 1: MA-2019-635N; Study 2: MA-2019-727N), and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All patients were informed about the study and provided written informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1:

Appendix A. English version of the final API-Uro questionnaire. Figure B.1. Urological cohort - pattern of missing values in the API-OV. Figure B.2. Urological cohort - pattern of missing values in the API-Uro. Figure B.3. Bladder cancer cohort - missing values in API-OV. Figure B.4. Bladder cancer cohort - pattern of missing values in API-Uro. Figure D.1. Scree plot from parallel analysis.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Büdenbender, B., Köther, A.K., Kriegmair, M.C. et al. Getting specific: participation preference in urooncological decision-making. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak 23, 114 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12911-023-02201-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12911-023-02201-8

Keywords